Tuesday 26 February 2013

Phobic Disorders - Psychodynamic Explanations of Phobic Disorders

AO1
AO2
Freud was the first to offer a psychological explanation for the origins of phobias. He proposed that a phobia was the conscious expression of repressed conflicts.

The ego deals with conflict by protecting itself and repressing the emotions into the unconscious. Freud demonstrated his explanation with the case study of Little Hans who had become terrified of horses pulling a laden cart.
Research support – therapies that simply target the symptoms of phobia, are not 100% successful, this is perhaps because they fail to deal with the underlying causes of the phobias.
There is a tendency in this approach to suggest that phobias can be reduced to a simple set of principles such as repressed anxieties. 

Therefore it is reductionist, but this means that other possibilities will not be looked at. It is likely that the ‘real’ explanations are likely to be a combination of a number of different explanations.
Hans had become terrified of horses pulling a laden cart. Freud suggested that Hans' phobia had developed for several reasons:

Firstly: Hans once heard a mad saying to a child "Don't put your finger to the white horse or it'll bite you". Hans once asked his mother if she would like to put her finger on his penis. His mother told him this would not be proper, leading Hans to worry that his mother might leave him. Hans projected once source of anxiety onto another - he became afraid of being bitten by a white horse, when in fact he was scared that his mother would leave him.

Secondly: Hans saw a horse with a laden cart fall down and thought it was dead. The horse symbolised his wish that his father would die and the laden cart symbolised his mother pregnant with his sister, and when it fell over this was like giving birth. Therefore, the laden cart symbolised his father dying and his mother giving birth - both events that filled him with anxiety.
Little Hans - Only one piece of evidence to support the explanation. This case study could just as easily be explained using classical conditioning.
Little Hans - Concerns one individual, so cannot be generalised to the wider population. Also, there is a lack of objectivity as both Hans’ father and Freud interpreted the evidence according to their expectations of the origins of phobias.

Sunday 10 February 2013

Phobic Disorders - Biological Explanations of Phobic Disorders

AO1
AO2
Genetic Factors
Family studies - Research shows that having a family member with a phobia increases the risk that an individual develops a similar disorder. (The family member who already has the disorder is called the proband)

Fyer et al (1995) - probands had three times as many relatives who also experienced phobias as the normal controls.

Solyom et al (1974) - 45% of phobic patients had at least one relative with the disorder, compared to a rate of 17% of non-phobic controls.
Family and twin studies - There is a considerable variability between disorders.
One of the problems with family and twin studies is that they fail to control for shared environmental experiences.
Behavioural inhibition - Kagan (1994) - identified an infant temperamental type that he described as 'behavioural inhibition' - infants who tend to withdraw from unfamiliar people, objects and situations. He suggested that this behaviour had a genetic basis.
Twin studies - There is a closer concordance between MZ twins than DZ twins, this is because they are genetically identical. This provides support for a genetic basis for phobic disorders.

Torgersen (1983) - compared MZ and same-sex DZ twin pairs where one twin had an anxiety disorder with panic attacks. Such disorders were five times more frequent in MZ twin pairs.
Behavioural inhibition - Longitudinal studies have followed children who showed signs of behavioural instability at birth. At primary school age such children were found to have higher ANS activity and also the largest number of specific fears. Similar results were found when looking at children whose parents suffered from panic disorder. Further follow-up studies found that both these groups of children developed significantly more anxiety disorders, supporting the hypothesis that behavioural inhibition to unfamiliar things or situations is genetically based and a risk factor for anxiety disorders.
The diathesis-stress model - Even at the highest rates it is clear that phobic disorders are not solely genetic and have other factors. This combination can be explained by the diathesis-stress model. This proposes that genetic factors predispose an individual to develop phobias but life experiences play an important role in triggering such responses.
An Evolutionary Approach
Ancient fears - Some stimuli are more likely to be feared than others, these could be referred to as ancient fears, in that these stimuli reflected very real fears to our ancestors.

Many other stimuli were also part of our ancestral environment, but because they posed no significant danger, are rarely feared. For the same reason, things that are dangers today rarely develop into phobias because they have not been around enough to have influenced our adaptive selection.
Clinical phobias - A lot of the research into prepotency and preparedness that we have looked at is concerned with avoidance responses rather than clinical disorders.
Studies of patients suffering from disabling disorders do not support the preparedness explanation.

In addition, research has found that clinical phobias do not display the suddenness of onset and resistance to treatment predicted by preparedness.
Cultural differences - There are significant differences in the kind of phobias reported by different cultural groups.

Brown et al (1990) - found that phobic disorders were more common among African American than white American participants even when socioeconomic factors were controlled. This shows that environmental/social factors are important in determining aspects of phobias.
Prepotency - Animals have evolved to respond to potential threats. Those ancestors who were able to respond appropriately to ancient threats were more likely to survive and pass on their genes to subsequent generations.

Preparedness - Seligman (1970) - argued that animals, including humans, are biologically prepared to rapidly learn an association between particular (potentially life-threatening) stimuli and fear. Once learned, this fear is difficult to get rid of.
Prepotency - Öhman and Soares (1994) - provided supporting evidence for prepotency effects. 'Masked' pictures were constructed of feared objects in such a way that the animals in the pictures were not immediately recognisable. Participants who were fearful of snakes or spiders showed greater GSR which indicates arousal of the ANS, when briefly shown 'masked' pictures compared to viewing neutral pictures or compared to normal participants. This shows that important components of phobic responses are set in motion before the phobic stimulus is presented in awareness, and these could be prepotent signs.
Preparedness - The two important predictions arising from the concept of preparedness are:
  • That we learn certain fears more readily
  • Such fears are harder to unlearn
McNally (1987) - concluded that although there was firm evidence for enhanced resistance to extinction of fear responses conditioned by 'prepared' stimuli, evidence for rapid acquisition of 'modern' phobias.

Tuesday 5 February 2013

Phobic Disorders - Issues of Reliability and Validity


AO1
AO2
Reliability refers to the consistency of a measuring instrument. Reliability can be measured in terms of:
  •           Inter-rater reliability
  •           Test-retest reliability
Inter-rater - Whether two independent assessors give similar scores.

Skyre et al (1991) - assessed inter-rater reliability for diagnosing social phobia by asking three clinicians to assess 54 patient interviews. There was high inter-rater agreement showing that the diagnosis of phobia is reliable.
Research evidence - Reliability has not always been found to be high.

Kendler et al (1999) - used face-to-face and telephone interviews to assess individuals with phobias. Over a one month interval, they found a mean agreement of +.46. Reliability over the long term was even lower at +.30.

Picon et al (2005) - however, found good test-retest reliability (better than +.80) with a Portuguese version of the SPAI over a 14 day interval. This indicates that reliability can be good at least in the short term.
Reasons for low reliability - Kendler et al (1999) - suggest that the low reliability found in their study might be due to several factors:

  • Test-retest reliability might be due to poor recall by participants of their fears, for example, people tend to over exaggerate fears when recalling previous distress.
  • Low inter-rater reliability might be due to the different decisions made by interviewers when deciding if the severity of a symptom does or does not exceed the clinical threshold for a symptom.
Test-retest - Whether the test items are consistent. Scales such as SCID take 1-2 hours to complete. The alternative is to use shorter, structured, self-administered scales. These are popular for specific phobias.

Hiller et al (1990) - reported satisfactory to excellent diagnostic agreement in a test-retest study using the MDC.
Cultural differences in diagnosis - Cultural norms about 'normal functioning' and 'normal' fears are likely to affect any diagnosis rendering the diagnosis 'invalid'.

Judgements may also vary with respect to culturally-relative disorders. A good example of this is provided by taijin-kyofusho (TKS), a culturally distinctive phobia recognised in Japan. This is a social phobia where an individual has a fear of embarrassing others in social situations.  An individual with such a condition would not be diagnosed in this country as having a social phobia, indicating the effect of cultural experiences on the diagnosis of a disorder.
Diagnosis by computer - Reliability may be improved through the use of computerised scales for assessing phobic and other disorders.

Kobak et al (1993) - suggest that increased reliability occurs because there is less opportunity for the administrator to affect th responses that are given. In addition people with social phobias may prefer to answer without the presence of another person because of their fears of negative evaluation.
Validity refers to the extent that a diagnosis represents something that is real and distinct from other disorders and the extent that a classification system measures what it claims to measure.

Comorbidity - Is an important issue for the validity of diagnosis. It refers to the extent that two or more conditions co-occur.

Research has found high levels of comorbidity between social phobias, animal phobias, generalised anxiety disorder and depression. Such comorbidity suggests that these conditions are not separate entities and therefore the diagnostic category is not very useful.
Comorbidity - The findings on comorbidity have been supported in many other studies.

Eysenck (1997) - reported that up to 66% of patients with one anxiety disorder are also diagnosed with another anxiety disorder. The implication is that a diagnosis should simply be 'anxiety disorder' rather than a phobia or OCD.
The implications of low reliability and/or validity - In order to conduct research on the effectiveness of treatments for phobic disorders researchers require a reliable and valid means of assessing the disorders in the first place.
Concurrent validity - Establishes the value of a new measure of phobic symptoms by correlating it with an existing one.

Herbert et al (1991) - tested concurrent validity of the Social Phobia Anxiety Inventory by giving the test and various other standard measures to 23 social phobics. The SPAI correlated well with the other measures.

Construct validity - Assesses the diagnostic questionnaires and interviews. This measures the extent that a test for phobic disorders really does measure a target construct of phobias.

To do this, clinicians identify possible target behaviours that we would expect in someone with a phobic disorder and see if people who score high on the test for phobic disorders also exhibit the target behaviour.

Support for concurrent validity - Mattick and Clarke (1998) - showed that their Social Phobia Scale (SPS) correlated well with other standard measures. This indicates that there are methods of diagnosis that agree and therefore appear to be measuring something real.
Support for construct validity - The SPAI correlates well with behavioural measures of social phobia. It also doesn't correlate with behaviours related to other anxiety disorders.
However, perhaps this is not surprising because the inventory includes questions about cognitions and behaviours across a range of fear-producing situations. This means that it is likely to correlate with behaviours associated with social phobias.

Sunday 3 February 2013

Addictive Behaviour - The Learning Approach

GAMBLING

INITIATION
  • Operant conditioning - any behaviour that produces a consequence that the individual finds rewarding then becomes more frequent. 
MAINTENANCE
  • Intermittent reinforcement - people continue to gamble because of the occasional reinforcement that is characteristic of most types of gambling. As a result, they become used to long periods without reward and their gambling behaviour is reinforced by the occasional payout.
  • Social approval - this type of behaviour may also be maintained because reinforcement is provided in the form of social approval from others.
  • Lambos et al (2007) - found that peers and family members of problem gamblers were more likely to approve of gambling.
RELAPSE
  • Conditioned cues - addicts learn (through classical conditioning) to associate other stimuli with their gambling behaviour. These stimuli act as triggers for gambling because they have the ability to increase arousal. If, after a period of abstinence, an individual comes into contact with one of these cues, they are at a higher risk of relapse.
  • Approach-avoidance conflict - because gambling has both positive and negative consequences for the individual, they are motivated to approach and to avoid situations where gambling is involved. This creates an approach-avoidance conflict, where motivation fluctuates between wanting to gamble and wanting to stop. Whether or not the gambler will gamble when faced with am urge to do so is related to their ability to control the increased arousal and delay their need for reinforcement.

Smoking

INITIATION
  • Availability of role models - Social learning theory explanations of experimental smoking propose that young people begin smoking as a consequence of the social models they have around them who smoke. From this perspective, experimental smoking is primarily a function of parental and peer role modeling and the vicarious reinforcement that leads to young people to expect positive physical and social consequences from smoking.
  • Popularity as a positive reinforcer - popularity among peers may also serve as a positive reinforcer in the initiation of smoking.
  • Mayeux et al (2008) - found a positive relationship between smoking at age 16 and boys' popularity two years later.
MAINTENANCE
  • Classical conditioning - the repetition of the act of smoking thousands of times a year eventually leads to a strong conditioned association between sensory aspects of smoking and the reinforcing effects of nicotine. Although the effects of nicotine in the brain are important when first starting smoking, smoking-related sensory cues rapidly become conditioned stimuli and so activate the same brain areas, making cessation more difficult.
RELAPSE
  • Conditioned cues - cues associated previously with receiving nicotine, such as the availability of cigarette smoke, increase the likelihood that the smoker will respond by smoking. 
  • Refusal self-efficacy - a concept related to the social learning theory explanation of smoking is self-efficacy, a person's belief in his or her ability to succeed in a particular situation. Among adults, those who smoke more frequently have less confidence in their ability to abstain and so are more likely to relapse.

Evaluation

  • Can't explain all forms of gambling - it is difficult to apply the same principles to all different forms of gambling.
  • Gender bias - most research is done on only one gender.
  • Culture bias - most research is done on individualistic cultures .
  • Conditioned cues - supporting evidence by Thewissen et al (2008) - tested the importance of environmental contexts in the urge to smoke. in one room, they repeatedly presented 33 smokers with a cue predicting smoking, whilst in a second room they presented a cue predicting smoking unavailability. Results supported the view that a cue predicting smoking later led to a greater urge to smoke than did a cue associated with smoking unavailability.
  • Treatment - Drummond et al (1990) - propose a treatment based on the idea that the cues associated with smoking or other forms of drug taking are an important factor in the maintenance of the habit. The treatment involves presenting the cues without the opportunity to engage in smoking.
  • Only some people get addicted - cannot explain social smokers or gamblers not getting addicted.
  • Different pathways - it has been suggested that there are different pathways for gambling that predict the likelihood of treatment being successful. People in the 'behaviourally conditioned' pathway are likely to have less severe gambling, so are more likely to consider treatment than the 'emotionally vulnerable gambler' who uses gambling primarily to relieve their aversive emotional states.
  • Significance of occasional reinforcement - learning explanations propose that people become 'hooked' on specific activities because they lead to a positive consequence. in real life, this consequence is likely to be occasional rather than consistent, as smoking a cigarette will not always produce a desired positive mood state or relieve a negative one.